I think we talked about this somewhere else already. @Maikl Jexocuha was that with you?@Ginny i'm wondering, is grammatical structure also based on phonetic ease in human language? In the sense that we partially build our grammar in our language based on the ease of pronunciation and emphasis on these phonetics? Eg. the way we
deliver a message to each-other verbally is always partially emotional, could grammatical structure enhance this delivery?
I guess it could fall into the prosody of a language, although that's also just the standard stress pattern of a language. It might be more at home in the field of pragmatics to define the deviation from the standard logically, but even so it's more descriptive than prescriptive.
Grammar was originally more prescriptive until syntax started to describe it.
Besides that, emotions are very flexible, and not as clearly defined as the relation between an object and a subject within spacetime. I know that some languages use words with certain affixes to define relationships between people. The lexicon is is somewhere between morphology and syntax, but this phenomenon is rather cultural than grammatical. Even so it merely scratches the surface of actual emotions. IMO, it is still pretty far from it.
Prosody is sort of the "melody" of a language. I haven't focussed much on prosody during my studies, so my definition is a bit wider than it probably is.A bit high level for me here, lol. Could you explain this part for me?
Mine will shut down next week, so I'll take the best I have when I have it. It's been years since I last discussed proper linguistics@Ginny woman, the amount of braincells I have to activate here to partake on this discussion....it's almost weekend.................
I'll take the effort to go through it, I promise...it's...a challenge lol. Complimentary.Mine will shut down next week, so I'll take the best I have when I have it. It's been years since I last discussed proper linguistics
I have no quirms. I don't know if it doesn't derail the thread unnecessarily, though.Hey @Ginny, I'm afraid I am having some difficulties understanding the whole context, too much of linguists that I don't understand well.
Would you mind if we go through each part?
Prosody is sort of the "melody" of a language. I haven't focussed much on prosody during my studies, so my definition is a bit wider than it probably is.
Certain words are expressed in a certain way based on syllable structure and syllabic word structure depending yet again on the morphosyntactical classification of each word within a sentence. For example, in English you could write a word the same way, but through stress you discern which category the word belongs to, either noun, verb or even adjective. Illustrative examples elude me right now. Thusly, [you] could view prosody on a variety of levels within the structure of a sentence, but this structure of a stress pattern will vary not only on levels within a sentence, which will stretch across the language in a repeating fashion to make it predictable in order to signify a specific meaning (e.g. to signify sarcasm). It will also vary across different languages on a grammatical level, but depending on language families they will likely be similar on the most superficial level.
Grammar can be seen in two different ways, prescriptive and descriptive. The way we learn grammar is prescriptive, as in there is a rigid structure that we must follow in order to communicate efficiently. On the other hand, syntax as the linguisitic subfield, is descriptive. It watches and describes grammar as a living thing rather than telling people how to use it. But it has its limitations. I do think syntax (and grammar in the process) is removed from emotion - the phrase structure of a sentence doesn't change based on what you're feeling. Nor does the word structure (morphology) change either. What can change is your use of it, which is distinguished through semantics and pragmatics. Semantics is concerned with meaning on the base level, the logical meaning of a sentence based on lexical meaning and morphosyntax alone. Pragmatics is taking it a step further into the actual use of language, analysing and describing the changes in semantic meanings based on context.
Interminglingly, you use of syntax influences the language not just on a pragmatic level. If you consider the phenomenon of the third hand, it can produce a domino effect which changes the syntax permanently. However, due to the prescriptive nature in which we learn and acquire language proficiency, grammar is the most stable structure and less likely to change within a single lifetime.
It's a bit hard using telepathy for asking someone to pass the salt
That's alright. A YT channel I watch has some lessons that might interest you.
If it's on my reply, I really have difficulties in understanding the concept.
But I rather want to start off with the parts that I think I understand than go "please can you rephrase the whole part?".
It's on my lack of my knowledge and understanding on linguistics. Many werds in there where I go ??? and have to look up.
@Ginny on that regard, I'll just try to learn the basics first, I guess. Feel free to answer, but I guess it will be better if I'll just research and try to understand everything first. @Professor Snep I am kind of annoyed at your post. But whatever.
That's alright. A YT channel I watch has some lessons that might interest you.
https://www.youtube.com/user/NativLang/playlists?view=50&sort=dd&shelf_id=9
It's probably my fault for being a bad teacher. I often speak as shortly as possible and keep forgetting that other people don't know the lingo.
If you're ready for me to reply to the questions (as I am sure I will be as annoyingly lingo-using as before), let me know.
Prosody is sort of the "melody" of a language. I haven't focussed much on prosody during my studies, so my definition is a bit wider than it probably is.
Certain words are expressed in a certain way based on syllable structure and syllabic word structure depending yet again on the morphosyntactical classification of each word within a sentence. For example, in English you could write a word the same way, but through stress you discern which category the word belongs to, either noun, verb or even adjective. Illustrative examples elude me right now. Thusly, [you] could view prosody on a variety of levels within the structure of a sentence, but this structure of a stress pattern will vary not only on levels within a sentence, which will stretch across the language in a repeating fashion to make it predictable in order to signify a specific meaning (e.g. to signify sarcasm). It will also vary across different languages on a grammatical level, but depending on language families they will likely be similar on the most superficial level.
rammar can be seen in two different ways, prescriptive and descriptive. The way we learn grammar is prescriptive, as in there is a rigid structure that we must follow in order to communicate efficiently. On the other hand, syntax as the linguisitic subfield, is descriptive. It watches and describes grammar as a living thing rather than telling people how to use it. But it has its limitations. I do think syntax (and grammar in the process) is removed from emotion - the phrase structure of a sentence doesn't change based on what you're feeling. Nor does the word structure (morphology) change either. What can change is your use of it, which is distinguished through semantics and pragmatics. Semantics is concerned with meaning on the base level, the logical meaning of a sentence based on lexical meaning and morphosyntax alone. Pragmatics is taking it a step further into the actual use of language, analysing and describing the changes in semantic meanings based on context.
Interminglingly, you use of syntax influences the language not just on a pragmatic level. If you consider the phenomenon of the third hand, it can produce a domino effect which changes the syntax permanently. However, due to the prescriptive nature in which we learn and acquire language proficiency, grammar is the most stable structure and less likely to change within a single lifetime.
You could hypothesise that languages evolved independently from a single specific structure that is based on how the human mind worked at the time (I had been hoping to find out more on this reading Pinker someday). Grammar can only structure what we know about the world around us, so it is invariably linked to the lexicon we develop in naming things. Thereby, we also structure words on a smaller scale by using grammatical units and perhaps even signifying them on the word itself by using a different morpheme or allomorph, in congruence with an affix. The names are moderately independent at first, probably having been the first to develop. That's how the lexicon is defined the most by culture. And words are the first things to change, locally speaking. The structure, however, is combining words on a less individual and less local if not (eventually) global scale. As languages spread, they changed in between villages and cities, but in order to communicate the meaning of (potentially) unknown words, grammar provides a gist about the grammatical relation between one object and another. It doesn't need to convey emotion at that level of evolution.
However, emotion is an intrinsically biological thing (at least recognisable among mammals). It could tie in with language on a different level, but what level would that be? It's how we use it that eventually conveys any emotional significance to an utterance, but it needs to be removed from both semantics, morphology and syntax, because they are the most basic blocks of meaning we have to make up a sentence or any utterance. They are the foundations we can build upon. To infringe on that distorts both the structure and the meaning, which isn't what we want to do, right? So we must add something to the utterance in order to singularise the colour of the meaning that we wish to bring across.
Heretofore, in the most known languages on the western hemisphere, it has been done through context and stress. But I am sure that there are different ways it can be brought across, perhaps even on a syntactical level, which would mean that emotions have been defined in such a way that they are universally fixed. But emotions are never that fixed, not even within a single person, to ever trickle down into language structure on a more permanent basis. A grammar would have to be infinitely complex to define every nuance to every emotion that a human is capable of experiencing to let it come to that. And that is not going to happen in a foreseeable future. It has to be conveyed through miniscule changes within a language that doesn't affect the sentence foundation too much to obscure the meaining. So it needs to be done on the levels of context and prosody*, and perhaps even the lexicon where it can't be done differently. These are the only measurable features with which we can discern emotions, and based on the languages structure and lexicon - which differ culturally - the basis of emotional conveyance within a language also differs on a cultural level.
(*At this point I remembered that tone is also important for the deliverance, but it sort of plays into prosody as well. What needs to be remembered is that there are also languages that make tone a morphological feature that changes the lexical meaning of a word.)
Took some effort, but I got through it, got a good amount of knowledge searching up as well, so it was fun! And mostly agree with everything stated thereI hope this wasn't too much. I feel like I went on a very big tangeant that wasn't entirely necessary. But it was good to dive into this realm of thought.
Edit: fixed some syntax errors
In Ancient Greek, there was a measure of where to stress which word based on its structure. I never got behind that, to be honest. In English, there is a specific stress pattern wrt word class and the number of syllables (vaguely remembered knowledge from years ago) - it can make the difference between regret (v) and regret (n). I theorise that there are specific patters that repeat between languages, more specifically, related languages, like German and English. Therefore, most of my native stress on specific words (pragmatically on a syntactic level rather than morphological, but that's a different story) is the same a native English speaker would use.Question: Hmm, what exactly do you mean with it will stretch across the language in a repeating fashion? Does it mean there is a set of fixed stress patterns in each language? I wonder if there exists a universal pattern to describe the sarcastic tone irrespective of the language it is spoken to.
It's what happens when you start doing something, like, take a certain shortcut, and another takes the same shortcut, and this gets repeated until it's an established route that's common knowledge to everyone. Sometimes the trace of a shortcut through the bushes or between trees is permanently visible on the treaded ground, hundreds of pairs of feet having laid the groundwork of rebellious shortcutting...Question: I have to ask, what is this phenomenon of the third hand?
I mean that we can only have words for what we know. And the way we think about these things decides how we talk about it, not just lexically, but structually as well. In our globalised world, it's not easy to see, but if you study more obscure languages, there may not be ones that have a culturally developed sense of left and right, but only use cardinal directions -- referencing the vague memory of reading Guy Deutscher's Through the Language Glass. And it's this way that language both structures our thoughts, and how our thoughts influence what we see around us - how we classify things in our minds.Question: Grammar can only structure what we know about the world around us , do you mean that grammar can only describe that what is around us by structuring the nouns, verbs, ... known to us? It's interesting that grammar could provide universal leverage towards the understanding of unknown words. Sounds logical though.
It may become a grammatical unit at some point, but as grammar is concerned with only the most foundational structure, delivery (wrt accents and dialects) is not covered by this field of study. This is what the parole part of linguistics is concerned with, syntax being on the langue side of linguistics. (The dichotomy of langue and parole was established by Ferdinand de Saussure and bisects linguistics into fields concerned with "structure" and "use" respectively.)Question: If grammatical structure and syntax in extent are laid down by a language's rule set, couldn’t there be a difference between languages that emphasise on emotional delivery? Let’s say southern or more vocal languages will have a stronger emphasis on intonation and overall prosody. You could perhaps see this within a wider cultural context (southern, western, etc.).
They are. And complicated af. Actually, I've been taught 5 tones in Mandarin. I know Japanese has two, iirc what my ex said. It's very interesting for sure and not something I have looked into very much.Tonal languages! These on their own are pretty interesting, rather melodic. Still have to check on these.
https://www.thoughtco.com/four-tones-of-mandarin-2279480
Interesting... go on.An agressive momentum
The cat got scared when a ball dropped in front of it. - Neutral
When the ball dropped in front of the cat, it got scared. - Syntax (placing action before emotion)
When the ball plunged in front of the mouser, it got frightened. - Context (exaggeration of verbs / nouns)
As the projectile plunged right ahead of the mouser, it got frightened. - Stress (adding stress to aggravate emotion)
The passing of a loving gesture
He gave her the flowers and told her he loved her. - Neutral
When he gave her the flowers, he told her he loved her. - Syntax (emphasis on the dual action, giving and telling affection on the moment)
When he handed her the blossom, he conveyed to her that he loved her. - Context (to smoothen the situation, add closeness to the situation)
While handing her the blossom, the gentleman conveyed her his love. - Stress (reduce stress to convey a smooth tonal note, emphases on him performing action)
Sorry for taking so long to reply, but I've been too preoccupied to find the concentration until now.
In Ancient Greek, there was a measure of where to stress which word based on its structure. I never got behind that, to be honest. In English, there is a specific stress pattern wrt word class and the number of syllables (vaguely remembered knowledge from years ago) - it can make the difference between regret (v) and regret (n). I theorise that there are specific patters that repeat between languages, more specifically, related languages, like German and English. Therefore, most of my native stress on specific words (pragmatically on a syntactic level rather than morphological, but that's a different story) is the same a native English speaker would use
It's what happens when you start doing something, like, take a certain shortcut, and another takes the same shortcut, and this gets repeated until it's an established route that's common knowledge to everyone. Sometimes the trace of a shortcut through the bushes or between trees is permanently visible on the treaded ground, hundreds of pairs of feet having laid the groundwork of rebellious shortcutting...
I looked it up: it's apparently known as the "invisible hand" theory.
I mean that we can only have words for what we know. And the way we think about these things decides how we talk about it, not just lexically, but structually as well. In our globalised world, it's not easy to see, but if you study more obscure languages, there may not be ones that have a culturally developed sense of left and right, but only use cardinal directions -- referencing the vague memory of reading Guy Deutscher's Through the Language Glass. And it's this way that language both structures our thoughts, and how our thoughts influence what we see around us - how we classify things in our minds.
It may become a grammatical unit at some point, but as grammar is concerned with only the most foundational structure, delivery (wrt accents and dialects) is not covered by this field of study. This is what the parole part of linguistics is concerned with, syntax being on the langue side of linguistics. (The dichotomy of langue and parole was established by Ferdinand de Saussure and bisects linguistics into fields concerned with "structure" and "use" respectively.)
They are. And complicated af. Actually, I've been taught 5 tones in Mandarin. I know Japanese has two, iirc what my ex said. It's very interesting for sure and not something I have looked into very much.
Interesting... go on.